富蘭克林效應

The Franklin Effect is a psychological phenomenon in which people like someone more after doing them a favor. The explanation for this is cognitive dissonance. People help others because they like others, even if they don't, because their minds have trouble maintaining logical consistency between their actions and perceptions.

In other words, the Franklin Effect is the result of an attack on a person's self-concept. Each person develops a persona that persists because inconsistencies in the personal narrative are rewritten, edited, and misunderstood.

Franklin's Effect Observation

Benjamin Franklin (for whom the effect is named) quoted what he called "an old adage": In his autobiography:

“People who have done you a good thing once are more likely to do it again than people who have done you a favor.”

Franklin explained how he dealt with hostility from rival legislators while serving in the Pennsylvania General Assembly in the 18th century:

Hearing that he had a very rare and curious book in his library, I wrote him a note expressing my desire to read the book and asking him to lend it to me for a few days. He sent it immediately and I sent it back about a week later with another note expressing my fondness for him strongly. When we next met in the House, he spoke to me very politely (something he had never done before). From that time he showed himself willing to serve me on every occasion, and we became good friends, a friendship which lasted until his death.

Research

Jecker and Landy conducted a study on this effect in 1969, in which students were invited to participate in a quiz contest run by the researchers, in which they could win a cash prize.

After this game is over,

  • One-third of the students who "won" did so. The researchers approached them and asked them to return the money on the grounds that he had used his own funds to pay the winners and now there was not enough money;
  • Another third were asked by the secretary to return the money because it belonged to the psychology department and there were insufficient funds;
  • Another third had no contact at all.

All three groups were then asked how much they liked the researcher.

  • The second group dislikes him the least
  • The first group dislikes him the least

This shows that refund requests from intermediaries decrease their favorability, while direct requests increase their favorability.

In 1971, University of North Carolina psychologists John Schopler and John Compere conducted the following experiment:

They had subjects take a learning test against accomplices posing as other students. Subjects were told that learners would watch the teacher use a stick to tap out long patterns on a series of wooden cubes. Learners are then asked to repeat these patterns. Each teacher will try two different methods on two different people, one at a time. During a round, the teacher provides encouragement when the learner masters the pattern correctly. In another round of experiments, teachers insulted and criticized students when they made mistakes. Afterward, teachers completed a situation report questionnaire that included questions about learner attractiveness and likability. Overall, subjects who were insulted were rated as less attractive than subjects who were encouraged.

The subjects' own behavior toward their accomplices influenced their perceptions of them - "You tend to like people who are nice to you and dislike people who are rude to you."

A newer but smaller study of Japanese and American subjects by psychologist Yu Niiya replicated the results.

Effects as a paradigm of cognitive dissonance

Franklin's view is cited as cognitive dissonance theory, which proposes that people change attitudes or behaviors to resolve tensions or "dissonances" between thoughts, attitudes, and actions. In the case of the Ben Franklin Effect, there is an inconsistency between a subject's negative attitude toward another person and their knowledge that they have done the other person a favor.

Other explanations

Psychologist Yu Niiya attributes this phenomenon to the requestee reciprocating the requester's perceived attempts to ignite rapport. This theory would explain the absence of the Ben Franklin effect when intermediaries are used.

use

In the world of sales, the Ben Franklin Effect can be used to build rapport with customers. Rather than offering unsolicited assistance to prospects, salespeople can ask prospects for assistance: "For example, ask them to share with you what they think are the most compelling product benefits, where they think the market is going, or which products might make you interested." Interested" a few years later. This kind of pure favor, if unrequited, can build goodwill, thereby enhancing your ability to win the customer's time and future investment. "

The Franklin Effect can also be observed in successful mentor-mentee relationships. One source noted that the relationship "is defined by a fundamental imbalance of knowledge and influence." Trying to proactively reciprocate may be counterproductive, as the role reversal and unsolicited help may put your mentor in an unexpected and awkward position.” The Ben Franklin Effect is referenced in Dale Carnegie's best-selling book How to Win Friends and Influence People . Carnegie interpreted requests for favors as "a subtle but effective form of flattery."

As Carnegie suggested:

...When we ask a coworker to do us a favor, we are expressing that we think they have something that we don’t have, whether it’s more wisdom, more knowledge, more skills, or something else. This is another way to show admiration and respect that others may not have noticed before. This immediately improves their opinion of us and makes them more willing to help us again because they enjoy our admiration and actually start to like us.

Psychologist Yu Niiya believes that the Ben Franklin Effect confirms Takeo Doi's "gan" theory, as described in "The Anatomy of Dependence." It states that dependent, childlike behavior can trigger a parent-child relationship in which one partner sees themselves as the caregiver. In effect, "Agan" creates a relationship in which one person feels responsible for the other, who is then free to behave immaturely and make demands.

One commentator discussed the Ben Franklin Effect in relation to dog training, arguing that "it's more about the human side of the relationship than it is about the dog itself." While trainers often distinguish between training methods based on positive and negative reinforcement impact on the dog, but it may also be relevant to "consider the impact both methods may have on the trainer." The Franklin Effect states that how we treat dogs during training affects how we view them as individuals, specifically how much we like or dislike them. When we do nice things for our dogs in the form of treats, praise, petting, and play that reinforce desired behaviors, the treatment may make us like them more. And if we use harsh words, collar yanking, or hitting to try to change our dog's behavior, then we will begin to like our dog less. "

reverse

The opposite happens when we start to hate someone for whom we have done something wrong. We dehumanize them in order to justify the bad things we do to them.

It has been suggested that if soldiers who kill enemy servicemen in combat later come to hate them, it is because this psychological strategy helps "reduce the dissonance of the killing." This phenomenon may also "explain long-standing feuds like Hatfield and McCoy"; or vendettas in different cultures: "Once started, we may not be able to stop and engage in behavior that we would not normally allow." As one As one commentator put it, "Prisoners came to look down upon prisoners; concentration camp guards came to treat prisoners inhumanely; soldiers coined derogatory terms for the enemy. It is difficult to hurt someone you admire. It is even harder to kill a fellow human being. Put yourself in the position of Treat the casualties you cause as something lower than yourself, something worthy of being harmed, so that you can continue to view yourself as a kind and honest person and continue to maintain your sanity.”

Review

All comments are moderated before being published

HealthyPIG Magazine

View all
牛骨湯食譜大全|Instant Pot 壓力煲 & 傳統老火湯版本

牛骨湯食譜大全|Instant Pot 壓力煲 & 傳統老火湯版本

牛骨湯食譜係香港家庭常見嘅煲湯之一,牛骨湯香濃滋補,配合中藥材更具養生功效。本文介紹肉骨類選擇、牛骨湯建議配搭、常見中藥材分類,以及Instant Pot壓力煲與傳統老火湯版本食譜,並引用科學研究支持。
澳洲飲用水發現「食腦變形蟲」:全球風險與地區對策(含各國/各州實用指南)

澳洲飲用水發現「食腦變形蟲」:全球風險與地區對策(含各國/各州實用指南)

澳洲飲用水發現「食腦變形蟲」:全球風險與地區對策(含各國/各州實用指南) 澳洲飲用水發現「食腦變形蟲」:全球風險與地區對策(含各國/各州實用指南) 重點:事件本身不代表飲水會感染;主要風險來自水經鼻腔進入。 目錄 ...
如何判斷雞翅是否變壞?

如何判斷雞翅是否變壞?

重點摘要 雞翅會變質嗎? 如何判斷雞翅是否變壞? 過期雞翅還能食嗎? 雞翅可存放多久? 如何儲存雞翅? 雞翅可以冷凍嗎? 結論 雞翅會變質嗎? 會。皮脂較多、表面不潔或溫度過高時,細菌繁殖更快。 如何判斷雞翅是否變壞? 外觀:皮色發黃、出黑斑或血水。 觸感:表面黏滑、軟爛。 氣...
如何判斷雞蛋是否變壞?

如何判斷雞蛋是否變壞?

重點摘要 雞蛋會變質嗎? 如何判斷雞蛋是否變壞? 過期雞蛋還能食嗎? 雞蛋可存放多久? 如何儲存雞蛋? 雞蛋可以冷凍嗎? 結論 雞蛋會變質嗎? 會。殼面有微孔,溫差及濕度變化會令細菌入侵。 如何判斷雞蛋是否變壞? 水測:沉底=較新鮮;浮起=多半變壞。 打開觀察:蛋白渾濁水樣、蛋黃...
如何判斷羊肉是否變壞?

如何判斷羊肉是否變壞?

重點摘要 羊肉會變質嗎? 如何判斷羊肉是否變壞? 過期羊肉還能食嗎? 羊肉可存放多久? 如何儲存羊肉? 羊肉可以冷凍嗎? 結論 羊肉會變質嗎? 會。脂肪多而易氧化,處理或存放唔好就會變壞。 如何判斷羊肉是否變壞? 顏色:紅轉黑,或出現綠斑。 脂肪:由白轉黃兼有酸味。 氣味:由輕...
如何判斷豬肉是否變壞?

如何判斷豬肉是否變壞?

重點摘要 豬肉會變質嗎? 如何判斷豬肉是否變壞? 過期豬肉還能食嗎? 豬肉可存放多久? 如何儲存豬肉? 豬肉可以冷凍嗎? 結論 豬肉會變質嗎? 會。豬肉表面水活度高,加上處理不潔或溫度過高,容易腐敗。 如何判斷豬肉是否變壞? 顏色:粉紅轉灰、發綠或出斑。 氣味:酸臭、腥臭味濃 ...
如何判斷牛肉是否變壞?

如何判斷牛肉是否變壞?

重點摘要 牛肉會變質嗎? 如何判斷牛肉是否變壞? 過期牛肉還能食嗎? 牛肉可存放多久? 如何儲存牛肉? 牛肉可以冷凍嗎? 結論 牛肉會變質嗎? 會。牛肉含高蛋白同水分,若溫度控制或衛生不當,細菌會快速繁殖,導致變壞。 如何判斷牛肉是否變壞? 顏色:鮮紅轉深褐甚至發黑;脂肪變黃。 ...
成年後懷疑自己有注意力不足過動症(ADHD),應該接受診斷嗎?

成年後懷疑自己有注意力不足過動症(ADHD),應該接受診斷嗎?

在過去,注意力不足過動症(Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, ADHD)多被視為「小孩的病」,很多成年人小時候從未被評估或診斷。直到近年社會對心理健康重視度提升,許多成人才開始懷疑,自己長期以來的專注困難、健忘、衝動或時間管理不良,可能與 ADHD 有關。這種「晚發現」的情況相當普遍,也引發了問題:成年後是否值得接受 ADHD 診斷?

哪些職業對健康影響最大?科學與現實的分析

哪些職業對健康影響最大?科學與現實的分析

在現代社會中,工作佔據了人們生命中相當大的一部分。然而,不同職業對健康的風險並不相同。一些工作性質或環境,會顯著增加慢性病、心理壓力、甚至縮短壽命的風險。以下從科學研究與醫學角度,探討幾類對健康損害較大的職業,並附上相關統計數據。